In March 2012 the Bolton Wanderers Football Club midfielder Fabrice Muamba collapsed on the field of play in a Premier League match against Tottenham Hotspur. He had suffered a heart attack and although he was apparently dead with no vital signs for a considerable length of time, he survived. In fact, he made a rapid recovery and was discharged from hospital a month later. The only sour note in what has been a remarkable return to health is the recent announcement of his enforced retirement from professional football.
As for the incident itself,
Fabrice Muamba is on
record describing his impressions of what happened. At first he
felt a surreal dizziness as though he was running along inside someone else's
body. The last thing he remembers is seeing two of the Tottenham player Scott Parker. Interestingly, he reports no feeling of
pain as the cardiac arrest occurred and unconsciousness took hold.
I can't be the only football
fan whose initial empathy for Fabrice Muamba and his family was the starting
point for a deeper reflection upon the episode. Obviously, it is shocking that
this could happen to a fit young 23 year old professional sports person. But
beyond that, it certainly put my own mortality into sharp perspective. Or put
bluntly, it made me think about my future death. When will it come? (Hopefully
not for many years!) What will be its circumstances? (Peaceful, I hope!). And
very simply - what does it feel like to die?
Death is a fundamental issue
for human beings. We are unique amongst all the creatures on earth in having
the cognitive capacity to possess the knowledge that one day we will die. This
means that we have to come to terms with our deaths as we go about our lives. I
am not an acquaintance of Fabrice Muamba and so I can't ask him about his
experiences first hand. Yet I have no doubt that doing so would provide
insights that would be useful in helping me to better come to terms with death.
However, as a psychologist,
I do have access to the vast library of psychological research that has been
carried out on many and varied topics. Though rare, cases of succesful
resuscitation following protracted periods of clinical death, where a patient’s
breathing and heart beat stop, do occasionally happen. One morning I decided to
search the psychological research database to see what studies had been carried
out with such survivors. One particular paper caught my eye for the Cool
Psychology blog.
Around the turn of the
millennium, Sam Parnia and his colleagues at Southampton General Hospital in
the south of England approached all the patients in the hospital that been
resuscitated following a heart attack in one year. These patients could easily
be identified thanks to the telephone switchboard operators who routinely log
all resuscitations as they are called in through the hospital's emergency phone
system. There were 63 such patients and they were all interviewed. This group
was ideal to form the basis of the study because cardiac arrest patients
exhibit two out of the three criteria required to pronounce an individual dead
- absence of cardiac output (no heartbeat), and absence of respiratory effort
(not breathing). In fact many also exhibit the third criterion of fixed dilated
pupils.
The research was in three
parts. In the first part, each patient was simply asked whether they had any
memories from when they were unconscious following their heart attack, and if
so to recount them as clearly as possible. They found that seven individuals
could recall their thoughts during the time they were unconscious, which is
eleven per cent of the full group.
"Near Death
Experience" is a recognised syndrome comprising of a specific pattern of
thoughts and memories experienced during an episode of clinical death. A
scoring system exists for classifying near death experience. Known as the
"Greyson
Scale", it consists of a list of consistently reported near
death experiences including, among others, feeling time speed up or slow down,
recalling scenes from the past, feelings of peace and harmony with the
universe. This first part of the Southampton research aimed to distinguish
between true near death experience and more random thoughts and memories during
the unconsciousness. The Greyson scale was used to assign a score to the
experiences recounted by each participant - the more experiences on the scale
that are recounted, the higher the given score.
The Southampton patient
group recounted several experiences during their unconsciousness. They
remembered coming to a point or border of no return, feelings of peace,
pleasantness and joy, heightened sensual awareness and a feeling of time
speeding up. Four of the patients were classified as having had a true near
death experience. Two others remembered some details commonly encountered
during near death experience but not quite enough to be classified in the first
group. The other patient recalled a less typical experience of people jumping
off a mountain.
One of the classic near
death experiences reported previously is the sensation of looking down at
oneself from a viewpoint close to the ceiling. This is known technically as an
"out of body experience". The second part of the study assessed out
of body experiences in a very simple yet very clever (and really cool) way. The
researchers suspended boards from the ceilings of all of the wards in the
hospital. These boards had writing and figures drawn on their upper sides that
would only be visible from a vantage point near the ceiling. Therefore, any
participants reporting an out of body experience could reasonably be asked to
describe what they saw on the upper sides of the boards as a test of whether
their experience was real or imagined.
That seems to me to be an
astonishing amount of trouble to have gone to in order to investigate this
phenomenon. But, still, it perfectly captures the true essence of what science
is - using logical tests to gather evidence as to whether some phenomenon is
true or not. Science is not all about lab coats, high tech gadgets and
undecipherable equations. These investigators simply and elegantly used very
low-tech painted boards to set about gathering evidence for out of body
experiences.
Unfortunately, though,
despite having gone to all the trouble of installing these aids, none of the
seven patients with memories from their period of unconsciousness reported
viewing themselves from above. Therefore, sadly, this unsophisticated but
ingenious research technique could not properly be put to use in the study.
The third part of the study
aimed to find out whether the body's physiological state during cardiac arrest
had any bearing on the patients being able to remember their thoughts while
they were unconscious. The researchers were allowed access to the patients'
medical notes and these recorded the levels of oxygen, sodium and potassium in
the blood during the resuscitation. To try and tease apart how these may have
been affecting the patients the study drew a comparison between the four
patients that were classified as having had a true near death experience, and
the 59 remaining patients in the study that did not. There was one very
interesting difference between the two groups - blood oxygen levels were higher
in the near death experience group of patients.
This is intriguing because
one of the leading theories of near death experience at the time was that
feelings such as heightened sensual awareness and time speeding up came about
due to a lack of oxygen in the brain. It was thought that these phenomena
occurred as the brain was starved of oxygen. The Southampton study instead
suggests that rather than a lack of oxygen, it is the opposite, an availability
of brain oxygen that seems necessary for a vivid near death experience. There
is a logic to this because better brain oxygenation would allow for improved
cognitive function during the resuscitation, which would explain the more vivid
experience and the ability to commit it to memory.
What a wonderful piece of
research. This paper is a worthy addition to the Cool Psychology blog because
it has an elegance of design that is to be admired. Following up all the cardiac
arrest sufferers at the hospital provides an insight into what proportion of
people will likely experience conscious thought in the moments leading up to
death - around ten per cent. It was certainly innovative to collect
physiological information as well as the interview responses. Thanks to that we
know that high blood oxygen levels during cardiac arrest are probably required
to support a level of brain activity sufficient to register and form memories
during the experience. But it was an inspired idea to fix message boards just
below ceiling height to seek evidence for out of body experience. It was just
unfortunate that none of the patients had an out of body experience, and so
nothing can be learned from this highly original approach.
The paper is doubly welcome
in the Cool Psychology blog because it gives some very well founded insights
into an under-researched but fundamental human question: What does it feel like
to die? Fascinatingly, cardiac arrest appears not to be traumatic - of those
patients that could remember the experience at all, none reported it as
unpleasant or painful. Moreover, memories that could be recalled point to a
joyful, peaceful experience accompanied by a pleasantly heightened sensual
awareness. These findings chime with Fabrice Muamba's re-telling of his
experiences during a prolonged cardiac arrest. He said that he felt no pain
whatsoever - just an odd, difficult to explain
feeling.
So, what does it feel like
to die? On this evidence, death by cardiac arrest seems to feel either like
nothing, or something pleasant and perhaps slightly mystical. There was
no evidence that having a cardiac arrest is painful. How reassuring. I take
comfort from the notion that death may not necessarily be something to be
feared. This being the case I think we can all rest easier as we carry on our lives in death's ever present, if perhaps now
slightly fainter shadow.
The reference and a link to
the full paper describing this study is included below. This paper was
published in the peer review academic journal named Resuscitation. You can read the abstract (a short summary) of the
article online for free by clicking on this link.
There is a charge for obtaining the full paper unless you are able to access a
library at a university or hospital with a subscription to the journal.
Otherwise you could try e-mailing the lead author, Sam Parnia and ask very
politely for a pdf copy.
i always want to know how it feels to die that's why every time i am depressed and i really want to give up i attempt to kill myself but somethings holding me that's why i can't do it..i know there are some explanations why we always feel that way specially if we are depressed..
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
DeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
DeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
DeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
DeleteHere's some sound advice for coping with depression provided by the UK's National Heath Service http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Depression/Pages/Introduction.aspx
ReplyDeleteKilling yourself is the worst sin and leads you to go to hell straight away with no option , so no matter what , never kill yourself
ReplyDeleteI disagree. If a mentally ill person kills him or her self with out knowledge of what they are doing then should they go to hell because they where sick. You clearly believe in a God. Is he kind. If so how could he send anyone to burn forever. What crime could I do that would justify never ending pain of the highest degree. No one should ever go to hell for anything. People don't do bad things they do good. People see things in diff. ways. A man steals bread from a baker. Yes he steals does that mean he is evil. No he might need that to live or feed his family because he can't get a job. people like you make me mad. And yes I believe in a God although I supect we might go to the same place of warship but my God is different from yours.
DeleteYea I feel dead ones from time to time. I would say don't do it. Hugs and strength Josefin
ReplyDeleteI dont believe killing yourself is the worst possible sin and will lead straight to hell. It doesnt say that in the Bible that I have found, and I refuse to believe that God is going to damn someone to Hell for the kind of depression that leads to suicide. Telling someone that who is fighting depression or contemplating suicide is just a fear tactic and not at all what that person needs to hear.....Marsha
ReplyDelete"Killing yourself is the worst sin and leads you to go to hell straight away with no option , so no matter what , never kill yourself"
ReplyDeleteAlthough I don't personally believe in suicide that is my own personal opinion and certainly don't go shoving it down depressed people's throats or try to scare them with silly superstition.
Depression should be treated by a professional as it is very serious illness. You will feel better about living after you have overcome your depression.
Where in the Bible does it say that you will go to hell if you commit suicide? Nowhere exactly. Thing is, if you add or take away from the word of God…that puts YOU in danger. So, you can't just say that without being wrong. If you give the verse for "Thou shalt not murder" Then how do you justify an eye for an eye? Thing is you can not say that suicide is a sin, because the Bible doesn't really talk about it. Also, there were times Israel was in battle and the enemy had the upper hand, so they fell on their swords. Still, the Bible doesn't really address suicide. It's not natural and it usually happens because a person is ill or like the Israelites, rather not die by the hand of their enemy. Murder, that's when you take another's life, to that the Bible is very clear. Have you lied? Well, you broke the commandments and that makes you a sinner. We all sin, but we have redemption through Christ.
ReplyDeleteWhen I had my second daughter in went into shock. Popped up to top of ceiling. What I remember is seeingmthendoctor put IV into my arm. I WOKE Up in my room hours later, they were gonna give me blood thank god they didn't because they didn't test for HIV then.
ReplyDeleteI love it how this guy writes a really good read on the science of death, and then someone comes in broadcasting suicide wishes. (Funny how truly suicidal people seem to die, whereas attention whores seem to be ten times as "suicidal" and yet survive.) Then, enter ninety-seven religious people who begin to debate the Christian science of suicide.
ReplyDeleteKudos to Dr. Stephens for providing a helpful link, congratulations to "Anonymous" for attempting to stick to topic, and to the author of this article -- very good read. I enjoyed it.
Really interesting article, thanks so much for your research!
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by a blog administrator.
ReplyDelete